0
Access to paid content on this site is currently suspended due to excessive activity being detected from your IP address 50.19.47.197. Please contact the publisher to request reinstatement.
We're unable to sign you in at this time. Please try again in a few minutes.
Retry
We were able to sign you in, but your subscription(s) could not be found. Please try again in a few minutes.
Retry
There may be a problem with your account. Please contact the AMA Service Center to resolve this issue.
Contact the AMA Service Center:
Telephone: 1 (800) 262-2350 or 1 (312) 670-7827  *   Email: subscriptions@jamanetwork.com
Error Message ......
Original Article |

The Influence of Distractors on Odor Identification FREE

Volker Gudziol, MD; Thomas Hummel, MD
[+] Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Smell and Taste Clinic, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, University of Dresden Medical School, Dresden, Germany.


Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2009;135(2):143-145. doi:10.1001/archotol.135.2.143.
Text Size: A A A
Published online

Objective  To investigate the impact of the use of more contrasted distractors on correct odor identification in patients with olfactory loss.

Design  Randomized, cross-over study.

Setting  University clinic.

Patients  Thirty patients with olfactory deficits.

Interventions  The olfactory function of the patients was evaluated by means of the “Sniffin’ Sticks” test battery.

Main Outcome Measures  The distractors of the Sniffin’ Sticks odor identification test (classic test) were modified, and more contrasted distractors were used (contrasted test), while the applied odorants were the same. All patients performed both the classic and the contrasted odor identification tests in a randomized sequence.

Results  Eighteen patients were hyposmic, and 12 were functionally anosmic. Odor identification was significantly better in the hyposmic patients than in the anosmic patients (P < .01). As predicted, hyposmic patients demonstrated a significant increase in correct odor identification in the contrasted test, while anosmic patients did not.

Conclusion  The use of more contrasted distractors in cued odor identification tasks can contribute to better discrimination of anosmic and hyposmic patients, which is highly valuable in a clinical context.

Figures in this Article

Several psychophysical tests have been validated for the assessment of olfactory loss, including the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test,1 the test of the Connecticut Clinical Chemosensory Research Center,2 and the “Sniffin’ Sticks” test.3,4 All these tests include, or are even solely based on, cued odor identification tasks. While an odor is presented, the patients choose an item from a list of distractors that best characterizes the smell. The distractors listed for each odor in the odor identification task of the Sniffin’ Sticks test are typically similar; eg, the list presented together with an “orange” odor contains the descriptors orange, blackberry, strawberry, and pineapple. It is apparent that the identification rate of an odor depends on the similarity between descriptors that are presented together. For example, the use of the descriptors garlic and onion in 1 list reduces the rates of correct identification for the odor of garlic to 46% in the “Sniffin’ Sticks” odor identification test,5 although garlic is an odor that is extremely common and familiar to the population studied.3

The idea behind the present study was that when more contrasted distractors are used, it should be easier for patients with olfactory loss to select the correct item, while functionally anosmic patients would demonstrate no change in correct odor identification. It was hypothesized that such modulation of an odor identification task would lead to better discrimination of patients with smell dysfunction in relation to their olfactory deficit. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to investigate the impact of more contrasted distractors on correct odor identification in patients with olfactory loss. It was expected that such manipulations should have a pronounced sex-related effect, as men are typically outperformed by women in terms of verbal abilities.6

The study was performed according to the Declaration of Helsinki on Biomedical Research Involving Human Subjects. Thirty patients with smell deficits were included in the study. All patients exhibited olfactory loss according to the threshold, discrimination, and identification measures (TDI score) obtained with the Sniffin’ Sticks test.4 Twelve patients (6 women and 6 men; mean [SD] age, 55 [17] years) were functionally anosmic (TDI score ≤15.0), and another 18 patients (10 women and 8 men; age, 63 [8] years) were hyposmic (TDI score >15.0-30.5 points).

Olfactory function was obtained in all patients by means of the Sniffin’ Sticks test kit, which comprises 3 tests of olfactory function: odor threshold, odor discrimination, and odor identification. Odor thresholds were determined for phenylethyl alcohol using a 3-alternative forced-choice task. Three pens were presented to the patients in a randomized order: one containing the odorant at 1 of 16 possible dilutions, and the other two containing solvent only. The patient's task was to find out which of the 3 pens smelled of the odorant, which had been presented at the beginning of the test as the highest of the 16 concentrations. A staircase paradigm was used to present triplets of pens to the patients every 20 to 30 seconds to avoid olfactory desensitization. The patients were blindfolded to prevent visual identification of the odor-containing pens. Correct identification of the pen that contained the odorant in 2 successive trials triggered a reversal of the staircase to the next lower odorant concentration, whereas a single incorrect identification triggered the reversal of the staircase to the next higher concentration. From a total of 7 reversals, the mean of the last 4 staircase reversal points was used as threshold estimate.7 The test of odor discrimination was performed using 16 triplets of odorants. The patients were presented with 3 pens: two containing the same odorant, and one containing a different odorant. Each patient's task was to identify the pen that smelled different; therefore, a 3-alternative forced-choice task test design was reapplied. The patients were again blindfolded to prevent visual detection of the target odor pens. They were allowed to sample each odor only once. The interval between presentations of odor triplets was at least 30 seconds. The interval between presentations of individual odor pens was approximately 3 seconds. For odor identification, 16 odors were presented in a randomized sequence. The patients were free to sample the odors as often as necessary in order to identify them from a list of 4 distractors. The experimenter presented odor pens separated by an interval of at least 30 seconds to prevent olfactory desensitization.8,9 Hereafter, this odor identification test will be called the classic odor identification test. Results from the 3 tests were added together to obtain a composite score, the so-called TDI score.

In an additional trial, 16 odors of the classic odor identification test were used, while more contrasted distractors were available (Table). An attempt was made to ensure that dissimilarity of the selected distractors corresponded with the classification of odors described by Saito et al.10 All applied distractors have been confirmed to be familiar to the population studied.3 Hereafter, this test will be called the contrasted odor identification test. The test interval between the classic odor identification test and the contrasted identification test was at minimum 30 minutes. Whether the classic or the contrasted odor identification test was performed first was randomly determined.

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable. Odors and Distractors in the Classic and the Contrasted Odor Identification Tests

Data were analyzed using SPSS 12.0 for Windows (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Illinois) and repeated-measures analysis of variance (within-subject factor classic/contrasted; between-subject factors anosmia/hyposmia and sex). Patient age was used as a covariate to account for age-related differences between groups. The α level was set at 0.05.

Hyposmic patients demonstrated better odor identification than anosmic patients (F1,25 = 15.0; P = .001). The hyposmic patients scored 3.2 (3.1) mean (SD) points higher in the contrasted odor identification test than in the classic odor identification test. Functionally anosmic patients exhibited only a minimal increase (0.2 [2.6] points) (Figure). The significance of this observation was emphasized by the interaction between factors classic/contrasted and anosmia/hyposmia (F1,25 = 7.93; P = .009). The factor sex had no significant effect (F1,25 < 0.01; P = .99).

Place holder to copy figure label and caption
Figure.

Number of correct odor identifications (range of score, 0-16) in the classic and contrasted odor identification tests, shown separately for anosmic and hyposmic patients. The boundary of the boxes closest to 0 indicates the 25th percentile; the bold line within the boxes marks the median; and the boundary of the box farthest from 0 indicates the 75th percentile. Whiskers above and below the box indicate the 90th and 10th percentiles, respectively. Dots indicate single measurements regarded as outliers. Note that there is significantly less overlap between the distributions of results from anosmic and hyposmic patients with the use of the contrasted test compared with the classic test.

Graphic Jump Location

Our findings showed that (1) odor identification was influenced by the distractors that were provided, and (2) the use of contrasted distractors resulted in a significant increase in correct odor identification in hyposmic patients but not in anosmic patients, both in absolute terms. Odor identification has been shown to be influenced whether it is performed as a cued or a free identification task.11 Even the color of the odorant has an impact on the verbal identification of the odor.12 Whether an odor is presented together with a verbal identifier or a photograph/pictogram that would show a graphical representation of the odor source3,13 also appears to make a difference. Therefore, it seems to be obvious that the choice of distractors in cued odor identification tasks may make correct odor identification more or less difficult. However, to our knowledge, none of the studies we reviewed involved patients with olfactory loss, and none of them systematically tried to exploit these effects for the improvement of olfactory diagnostics.

The use of more contrasted distractors led to an increase in correct odor identification. As hypothesized, this effect was significant in hyposmic patients but negligible in functionally anosmic patients. This result is apparent because the use of more contrasted distractors makes it easier for patients with incomplete olfactory loss to select the correct item. In contrast, odor identification in anosmic subjects does not seem to benefit from the use of contrasted distractors. In fact, odor identification scores in anosmic patients did not change in relation to the difficulty of the task. Therefore, the use of more contrasted odor identification tests could help to differentiate between hyposmic and functionally anosmic patients. This distinction seems to be of great clinical value, as patients with some olfactory function have a higher chance for recovery than patients with complete anosmia.14

In cued odor identification tasks, women usually outperform men,1 which is partly because women typically have better verbal abilities than men.6 In the present study, however, no such relationship between sex and odor identification performance was found. Even among the hyposmic patients, the increase of correctly identified odors in the contrasted test was not sex related. Therefore, although the sample size was relatively small, it could be hypothesized that the verbal tasks were too simple, so no sex-related differences were apparent.

In conclusion, the selection of distractors in cued odor identification tasks influences the results of the tests. This effect can be used to better differentiate between hyposmic and anosmic patients.

Correspondence: Thomas Hummel, MD, Smell and Taste Clinic, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, University of Dresden Medical School, Fetscherstrasse 74, 01307 Dresden, Germany (thummel@mail.zih.tu-dresden.de).

Submitted for Publication: November 7, 2007; final revision received April 1, 2008; accepted April 21, 2008.

Author Contributions: Dr Gudziol had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: Gudziol and Hummel. Acquisition of data: Gudziol. Analysis and interpretation of data: Gudziol and Hummel. Drafting of the manuscript: Gudziol and Hummel. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Hummel. Statistical analysis: Gudziol and Hummel. Obtained funding: Hummel. Administrative, technical, and material support: Hummel. Study supervision: Hummel.

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

Additional Contributions: Monika Roesner and Silvia Wolff-Stephan helped in collecting the data for this study.

Doty  RLShaman  PDann  M Development of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test: a standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function. Physiol Behav 1984;32 (3) 489- 502
PubMed Link to Article
Cain  WS Testing olfaction in a clinical setting. Ear Nose Throat J 1989;68 (4) 316, 322- 328
PubMed
Hummel  TSekinger  BWolf  SRPauli  EKobal  G “Sniffin’ sticks”: olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem Senses 1997;22 (1) 39- 52
PubMed Link to Article
Kobal  GKlimek  LWolfensberger  M  et al.  Multicenter investigation of 1,036 subjects using a standardized method for the assessment of olfactory function combining tests of odor identification, odor discrimination, and olfactory thresholds. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol 2000;257 (4) 205- 211
PubMed Link to Article
Hummel  TKonnerth  CGRosenheim  KKobal  G Screening of olfactory function with a four-minute odor identification test: reliability, normative data, and investigations in patients with olfactory loss. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol 2001;110 (10) 976- 981
PubMed
Larsson  MNilsson  LGOlofsson  JKNordin  S Demographic and cognitive predictors of cued odor identification: evidence from a population-based study. Chem Senses 2004;29 (6) 547- 554
PubMed Link to Article
Ehrenstein  WHEhrenstein  A Psychophysical methods. Windhorst  UJohans-son  HModern Techniques in Neuroscience Research. New York, NY Springer-Verlag NY Inc1999;1211- 1241
Hummel  TKnecht  MKobal  G Peripherally obtained electrophysiological responses to olfactory stimulation in man: electro-olfactograms exhibit a smaller degree of desensitization compared with subjective intensity estimates. Brain Res 1996;717 (1-2) 160- 164
PubMed Link to Article
Köster  EPde Wijk  RA Olfactory adaptation. Laing  DGDoty  RLBreipohl  WThe Human Sense of Smell. New York, NY Springer-Verlag NY Inc1991;199- 215
Saito  SAyabe-Kanamura  SKobayakawa  TKuchinomachi  YTakashima  Y A smell test based on odor recognition by Japanese people, and its application. Bell  GAWatson  AJTastes & Aromas The Chemical Senses in Science and Industry. Sydney, Australia University of New South Wales Press1999;75- 82
de Wijk  RACain  WS Odor quality: discrimination versus free and cued identification. Percept Psychophys 1994;56 (1) 12- 18
PubMed Link to Article
Zellner  DABartoli  AMEckard  R Influence of color on odor identification and liking ratings. Am J Psychol 1991;104 (4) 547- 561
PubMed Link to Article
Kobayashi  MImanishi  YIshikawa  M  et al.  Influence of visual information and test paradigm on clinical olfactory test results. Auris Nasus Larynx 2008;35 (1) 53- 60
PubMed Link to Article
Mori  JAiba  TSugiura  M  et al.  Clinical study of olfactory disturbance. Acta Otolaryngol Suppl 1998;538197- 201
PubMed

Figures

Place holder to copy figure label and caption
Figure.

Number of correct odor identifications (range of score, 0-16) in the classic and contrasted odor identification tests, shown separately for anosmic and hyposmic patients. The boundary of the boxes closest to 0 indicates the 25th percentile; the bold line within the boxes marks the median; and the boundary of the box farthest from 0 indicates the 75th percentile. Whiskers above and below the box indicate the 90th and 10th percentiles, respectively. Dots indicate single measurements regarded as outliers. Note that there is significantly less overlap between the distributions of results from anosmic and hyposmic patients with the use of the contrasted test compared with the classic test.

Graphic Jump Location

Tables

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable. Odors and Distractors in the Classic and the Contrasted Odor Identification Tests

References

Doty  RLShaman  PDann  M Development of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test: a standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function. Physiol Behav 1984;32 (3) 489- 502
PubMed Link to Article
Cain  WS Testing olfaction in a clinical setting. Ear Nose Throat J 1989;68 (4) 316, 322- 328
PubMed
Hummel  TSekinger  BWolf  SRPauli  EKobal  G “Sniffin’ sticks”: olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem Senses 1997;22 (1) 39- 52
PubMed Link to Article
Kobal  GKlimek  LWolfensberger  M  et al.  Multicenter investigation of 1,036 subjects using a standardized method for the assessment of olfactory function combining tests of odor identification, odor discrimination, and olfactory thresholds. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol 2000;257 (4) 205- 211
PubMed Link to Article
Hummel  TKonnerth  CGRosenheim  KKobal  G Screening of olfactory function with a four-minute odor identification test: reliability, normative data, and investigations in patients with olfactory loss. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol 2001;110 (10) 976- 981
PubMed
Larsson  MNilsson  LGOlofsson  JKNordin  S Demographic and cognitive predictors of cued odor identification: evidence from a population-based study. Chem Senses 2004;29 (6) 547- 554
PubMed Link to Article
Ehrenstein  WHEhrenstein  A Psychophysical methods. Windhorst  UJohans-son  HModern Techniques in Neuroscience Research. New York, NY Springer-Verlag NY Inc1999;1211- 1241
Hummel  TKnecht  MKobal  G Peripherally obtained electrophysiological responses to olfactory stimulation in man: electro-olfactograms exhibit a smaller degree of desensitization compared with subjective intensity estimates. Brain Res 1996;717 (1-2) 160- 164
PubMed Link to Article
Köster  EPde Wijk  RA Olfactory adaptation. Laing  DGDoty  RLBreipohl  WThe Human Sense of Smell. New York, NY Springer-Verlag NY Inc1991;199- 215
Saito  SAyabe-Kanamura  SKobayakawa  TKuchinomachi  YTakashima  Y A smell test based on odor recognition by Japanese people, and its application. Bell  GAWatson  AJTastes & Aromas The Chemical Senses in Science and Industry. Sydney, Australia University of New South Wales Press1999;75- 82
de Wijk  RACain  WS Odor quality: discrimination versus free and cued identification. Percept Psychophys 1994;56 (1) 12- 18
PubMed Link to Article
Zellner  DABartoli  AMEckard  R Influence of color on odor identification and liking ratings. Am J Psychol 1991;104 (4) 547- 561
PubMed Link to Article
Kobayashi  MImanishi  YIshikawa  M  et al.  Influence of visual information and test paradigm on clinical olfactory test results. Auris Nasus Larynx 2008;35 (1) 53- 60
PubMed Link to Article
Mori  JAiba  TSugiura  M  et al.  Clinical study of olfactory disturbance. Acta Otolaryngol Suppl 1998;538197- 201
PubMed

Correspondence

CME
Also Meets CME requirements for:
Browse CME for all U.S. States
Accreditation Information
The American Medical Association is accredited by the Accreditation Council for Continuing Medical Education to provide continuing medical education for physicians. The AMA designates this journal-based CME activity for a maximum of 1 AMA PRA Category 1 CreditTM per course. Physicians should claim only the credit commensurate with the extent of their participation in the activity. Physicians who complete the CME course and score at least 80% correct on the quiz are eligible for AMA PRA Category 1 CreditTM.
Note: You must get at least of the answers correct to pass this quiz.
Please click the checkbox indicating that you have read the full article in order to submit your answers.
Your answers have been saved for later.
You have not filled in all the answers to complete this quiz
The following questions were not answered:
Sorry, you have unsuccessfully completed this CME quiz with a score of
The following questions were not answered correctly:
Commitment to Change (optional):
Indicate what change(s) you will implement in your practice, if any, based on this CME course.
Your quiz results:
The filled radio buttons indicate your responses. The preferred responses are highlighted
For CME Course: A Proposed Model for Initial Assessment and Management of Acute Heart Failure Syndromes
Indicate what changes(s) you will implement in your practice, if any, based on this CME course.

Multimedia

Some tools below are only available to our subscribers or users with an online account.

608 Views
10 Citations
×

Related Content

Customize your page view by dragging & repositioning the boxes below.

Articles Related By Topic
Related Collections
PubMed Articles
Jobs
JAMAevidence.com

The Rational Clinical Examination: Evidence-Based Clinical Diagnosis
Vaginitis

The Rational Clinical Examination: Evidence-Based Clinical Diagnosis
Original Article: What Is Causing This Patient's Vaginal Symptoms?